Get the picture? Art in the brain of the beholder
- 17 July 2012 by Kat Austen
- Magazine issue 2873. Subscribe and save (New Scientist)
- For similar stories, visit the Books and Art and The Human Brain Topic Guides
STANDING in front of Jackson Pollock's Summertime: Number 9A
one day, I was struck by an unfamiliar feeling. What I once considered
an ugly collection of random paint splatters now spoke to me as a joyous
celebration of movement and energy, the bright yellow and blue bringing
to mind a carefree laugh.
It was my road-to-Damascus moment -
the first time a piece of abstract art had stirred my emotions. Like
many people, I used to dismiss these works as a waste of time and
energy. How could anyone find meaning in what looked like a collection
of colourful splodges thrown haphazardly on a 5.5-metre-wide canvas? Yet
here I was, in London's Tate Modern gallery, moved by a Pollock.
Since then, I have come to appreciate
the work of many more modern artists, who express varying levels of
abstraction in their work, in particular the great Piet Mondrian, Paul
Klee, and contemporary artist Hiroshi Sugimoto.
Even so, when I tried to explain my taste, I found myself lost for
words. Why are we attracted to paintings and sculptures that seem to
bear no relation to the physical world?
Little did I know that researchers
have already started to address this question. By studying the brain's
responses to different paintings, they have been examining the way the
mind perceives art. Although their work cannot yet explain the nuances
of our tastes, it has highlighted some of the unique ways in which these
masterpieces hijack the brain's visual system.
The studies are part of an emerging discipline called neuroaesthetics, founded just over 10 years ago by Semir Zeki
of University College London. The idea was to bring scientific
objectivity to the study of art, in an attempt to find neurological
bases for the techniques that artists have perfected over the years. It
has already offered insights into many masterpieces.
The blurred imagery of Impressionist paintings seems to tickle the
brain's amygdala, for instance, which is geared towards detecting
threats in the fuzzy rings of our peripheral vision. Since the amygdala
plays a crucial role in our feelings and emotions, that finding might
explain why many people find these pieces so moving.
Could the same approach tell us
anything about the controversial pieces that began to emerge from the
tail end of Impressionism more than 100 years ago? Whether it is
Mondrian's rigorously geometrical, primary-coloured compositions, or
Pollock's controversial technique of dripping paint onto the canvas in
seemingly haphazard patterns, the defining characteristic of modern art
has been to remove almost everything that could be literally
interpreted.
Although these works often sell for whopping sums of money - Pollock's No. 5
fetched $140 million in 2006 - they have attracted many sceptics, who
claim that modern artists lack the skill or competence of the masters
before them. Instead, they see the newer works as a serious case of the
emperor's new clothes, believing that people might claim to like them
simply because they are in fashion. In the scathing words of the
American satirist Al Capp, they are the "product of the untalented, sold
by the unprincipled to the utterly bewildered".
Chimp or Rothko?
We certainly do have a strong tendency
to follow the crowd. When asked to make simple perceptual decisions
such as matching up a shape with its rotated image, for instance, people
will often choose a definitively wrong answer if they see others doing
the same. It is easy to imagine that the herd mentality would have an
even greater impact on a fuzzy concept like art appreciation, where
there is no right or wrong answer.
Angelina Hawley-Dolan
of Boston College, Massachusetts, responded to this debate by designing
a fun experiment that played with her volunteers' expectations of the
pieces they were seeing. Their task was simple. The volunteers viewed
pairs of paintings - either the creations of famous abstract artists or
the doodles of amateurs, infants, chimps and elephants. Then they had to
judge which they preferred. A third of the paintings were given no
captions, while the rest were labelled. The twist was that sometimes the
labels were mixed up, so that the volunteers might think they were
viewing a chimp's messy brushstrokes when they were actually seeing an
expressionist piece by Mark Rothko. Some sceptics might argue that it is
impossible to tell the difference - but in each set of trials, the
volunteers generally preferred the work of the well-accepted human
artists, even when they believed it was by an animal or a child (Psychological Science, vol 22, p 435). Somehow, it seems that the viewer can sense the artist's vision in these paintings, even if they can't explain why.
With this in mind, I recently wandered
down to an art exhibition at University College London's Grant Museum,
where chimp and elephant art is exhibited alongside artworks by abstract
artist Katharine Simpson, whose blocky pieces are reminiscent of
Rothko's, but on a smaller scale and with more variety of colour.
Challenging myself to guess the professional work, I wandered the
gallery looking at the paintings before reading the captions. I managed
to get it right every time.
So a liking for abstract art can't be
explained by peer pressure. Yet Hawley-Dolan's experiment didn't explain
how we detect the hand of the human artist, nor the reason why the
paintings appeal to us. With a realistic picture, we might relate to the
expression on a person's face, or we might find symbolism in a still
life. But how does the artist hold our attention with an image that
bears no likeness to anything in the real world?
Of course, each artist's unique style
will speak to us in a different way, so there can be no single answer.
Nevertheless, a few studies have tackled the issue from various angles.
Robert Pepperell, an artist based at Cardiff University, UK, for
instance, has worked with Alumit Ishai of the University of Zurich,
Switzerland, to understand the way we process ambiguous figures. They
may look familiar, but "don't add up to something immediately
recognisable", Pepperell says. Like the work of Wassily Kandinsky or
certain pieces by Gerhard Richter, Pepperell's paintings, which sometimes take the composition of older masterpieces (see images here and here), are not entirely abstract but neither can they be readily interpreted like a representational painting.
Mind games
In one study, Pepperell and Ishai
asked volunteers to decide whether they saw anything familiar in the
piece. In a quarter of the cases they claimed to recognise something
real, even when there was nothing definite to pick out. They also had to
judge how "powerful" they considered the artwork to be. It turned out
that the longer they took to answer these questions, the more highly
they rated the piece under scrutiny. And this delay seems to be filled
with widespread neural activity, as revealed by later fMRI scans. From
these results, you could conclude
that the brain sees these images as a puzzle - it struggles to "solve"
the image, and the harder it is to decipher the meaning, the more
rewarding we find that moment of recognition (Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol 5, p 1).
This doesn't necessarily tell us much
about the more abstract work of Rothko, Pollock or Mondrian, since these
artists do not offer even the merest glimpse of a recognisable object
for the brain to latch on to. But they may instead catch our attention
through particularly well-balanced compositions that appeal to the
brain's visual system.
Consider the art of Mondrian, whose
work consists exclusively of horizontal and vertical lines encasing
blocks of colour. (His conviction in this principle was so strong that
fellow artist Theo van Doesburg's decision to use diagonal lines ended
their friendship.) Mondrian's art is deceptively simple, but
eye-tracking studies confirm that the patterns are meticulously
composed, and that simply rotating a piece radically changes the way we
view it. In the originals, the volunteers' eyes tended to linger for
longer on certain places in the image, but in the rotated versions they
would flit across the piece more rapidly. As a result, they considered
the altered images less pleasurable when they later rated the work (Journal of Vision, vol 7, p 1445).
Changing Mondrian's colours has a similar effect: in one example, a
large square of red in one corner is offset by a small dark blue square
on the opposite side, which contrasts more strongly with the surrounding
white. When the researchers swapped these colours, it threw off the
balance, leading the volunteers to take less enjoyable journeys around
the piece (Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol 5, p 98).
The same is probably true of many
other works. Oshin Vartanian at the University of Toronto, Canada, for
example, recently asked volunteers to compare a series of original
paintings to a set in which the composition had been altered by moving
objects around within the frame. He found that almost everyone preferred
the original, whether it was a still-life painting by Vincent van Gogh
or Joan Miró's abstract Bleu I.
What's more, Vartanian found that
manipulating the objects reduced activation in areas of the brain linked
with meaning and interpretation (NeuroReport, vol 15, p 893).
The results suggest that our mind notes the careful arrangements and
senses the intention behind the paintings, even if we are not
consciously aware of the fact. It is unlikely, to say the least, that
the chimps or children would ever hit upon such carefully considered
structures. That may explain why the volunteers in Hawley-Dolan's study
tended to prefer the work of the experienced artists.
Besides the balance of the
composition, we may also be drawn in by pieces that hit a sweet spot in
the brain's ability to process complex scenes, says Alex Forsythe,
a psychologist at the University of Liverpool, UK. She used a
compression algorithm to judge the visual complexity of different pieces
of art. The program tries to find shortcuts to store an image in the
smallest number of bits - the more complex the piece, the longer the
string of digits used to store the painting on the hard drive, offering a
more objective measure that human judgement. The results suggested that
many artists - from Edouard Manet to Pollock - used a certain level of
detail to please the brain. Too little and the work is boring, but "too
much complexity results in a kind of perceptual overload", says
Forsythe.
Art imitating life
What's more, many pieces showed signs
of fractal patterns - repeating motifs that reoccur at different scales,
whether you zoom in or zoom out of a canvas (British Journal of Psychology, vol 102, p 49).
Fractals are common throughout nature - you can see them in the jagged
peaks of a mountain or the unfurling fronds of the fern. It is possible
that our visual system, which evolved in the great outdoors, finds it
easier to process these kinds of scenes. The case for this theory is not
watertight, though, since the fractal content in the paintings was
considerably higher than you would normally find in natural scenes - to
the point that, in other circumstances, it would be considered too busy
to be pleasant. Forsythe thinks that the artists may choose their
colours to "soothe a negative experience we would normally have when
encountering too high a fractal content".
It's still early days for the field of
neuroaesthetics - and these studies are probably only a taste of what
is to come. It is intriguing, for example, that some scans have
registered the brain processing movement when we see a handwritten
letter. It is as if we are replaying the writer's moment of creation in
our brain. That may be down to our mirror neurons, which are known to
mimic others' actions. The results have led some to ponder whether the
work of Pollock might feel so dynamic because the brain reconstructs the
energetic movements the artist used as he painted. This hypothesis will
need to be thoroughly tested, though (Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol 11, p 197).
Others have speculated that we could use neuroaesthetic studies to
understand the longevity of some pieces of artwork. While the fashions
of the time might shape what is currently popular, works that are best
adapted to our visual system may be the most likely to linger once the
trends of previous generations have been forgotten (Spatial Vision, vol 21, p 347).
It would be foolish to try to reduce
art appreciation to a set of scientific laws, of course. While the
research helps us to understand the way the technique used to create an
artwork may appeal to the brain's visual system, an artist's ingenuity
also depends on their ability to develop or confront the ideas of those
before them. Benno Belke,
a German sound artist and cognitive scientist, points out that we
shouldn't underestimate the importance of recognising the style of a
particular artist and understanding their place in art history or the
culture of their time, for instance. This is where expertise comes in to
help you appreciate works you would have never enjoyed before, he
argues. Pepperell agrees. "Art is about heightened sensitivity and
awareness," he says. "If you're a connoisseur, you'll be looking for
subtleties."
Science can offer another stopping
point on this journey of understanding. "Art gives us knowledge about
the world. Some is emotional knowledge, some is the knowledge held by
the creator and for the neurobiologist it gives us a means of
understanding how the brain is organised," says Zeki.
Abstract art offers both a challenge
and the freedom to play with different interpretations. In some ways,
it's not so different to science, where we are constantly looking for
patterns and decoding meaning so that we can view and appreciate the
world in a new way.
The duchamp Code
How do we decode images to give them meaning? Context is crucial, particularly when viewing postmodern, ready-made art like Marcel Duchamp's famous installation, Fountain, which placed a urinal in an exhibition - much to the dismay of the artistic establishment.Kristian Tylén at Aarhus University in Denmark recently looked at the way the setting of an object can alter our perception of its message. He found that an understanding of intent activates areas in the right hemisphere of the brain that are traditionally associated with linguistic understanding. This holds whether that intent is conveyed through unusual incongruities in the image, like seeing a urinal in a gallery, or through use of accepted symbols such as a bunch of flowers by a doorway. It is almost as if we "read" the unusual arrangements in the same way that we read meaning in the arbitrary signs of a language.
No comments:
Post a Comment